A Daily Dose of Vitamin D May Preserve Cognition in Ageing
The
prevalence of dementia is increasing in line with the ageing of the UK
population, making research into potential preventative measures fundamental in
preserving quality of life for the elderly. It is frequently suggested that
vitamin D may reduce risk of neurodegenerative diseases [1], yet a recent study by Olsson et
al. found no association between vitamin D and incident dementia or cognitive
impairment [2].
From evaluating the wider literature, particularly considering the quality of
the published research, it has been found that there is not sufficient
evidence to make a definite conclusion. Nonetheless, it could be said to be
beneficial for all older adults to take a daily vitamin D supplement to prevent
deficiency, which would have adverse effects on skeletal health, resulting in
functional decline. This action may allow for neuroprotective effects attributed to vitamin D to also be obtained.
Dementia is a term that describes a set
of symptoms that result from damage to the brain. The most common cause is Alzheimer’s
disease (AD), but other dementias include vascular dementia, dementia with Lewy
bodies and frontotemporal dementia [3]. Symptoms vary between dementias, but
can be categorised as cognitive impairment, affecting memory, language,
attention, thinking, orientation, calculation and problem-solving; psychiatric
or behavioural disturbances, with changes in personality, emotional control, social
behaviours, depression, agitation, hallucinations and delusions; and
difficulties with activities of daily living (ADL) including driving, shopping,
eating and dressing. Progressive degeneration tends to occur, with an increase
in symptom severity over time and a gradual loss of independence [4], resulting in a
significant health and social care burden and a reduction in quality of life
for sufferers.
Age is the most common risk factor for
the development of neurodegenerative diseases [5] so an increase in life expectancy predisposes
individuals to dementia. Consequently, prevalence in the UK is predicted to
rise from 850,000 in 2015 to over 1 million by 2025 [6]. Current treatments available are
limited; they only help with the management of symptoms but do not prevent the
onset of the causative diseases [3]. This makes research into potential mechanisms to slow
cognitive decline and reduce incident dementia is invaluable.
Consumption of oily fish is frequently
associated with brain health due to its omega-3 fatty acid content. However,
studies investigating dietary supplementation with omega-3 have yielded
variable results. It has therefore been suggested that other components of oily
fish may also contribute to preservation of cognitive function and be a
modulating factor that explains the conflicting data [7]. One nutrient of particular
interest is vitamin D.
Vitamin D can be obtained from both
endogenous synthesis in the skin, where 7-dehydrocholesterol isomerises to
cholecalciferol upon exposure to UVB radiation [1], or from dietary sources such as oily
fish and fortified dairy products [8]. Hydroxylation in the liver by
25-hydroxylase forms 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) [9], which is biologically inert until
activation (5) by 1-α-hydroxylase to calcitriol
(1,25(OH)2D) [8].
1,25(OH)2D has a genomic effect by binding to nuclear vitamin D
receptors (VDRs) which interact with retinol X receptors to form heterodimers
that bind to vitamin D response elements on DNA initiating a cascade that
modulates gene transcription, and also has a non-genomic effect by binding to
non-nuclear VDRs [5].
The
primary role of vitamin D is in calcium and phosphorous homeostasis,
stimulating an increase in calcium absorption in the intestines by calbindin
synthesis and promoting deposition in bones [9]. However, VDRs are ubiquitous throughout the body and a
number of tissues have 1-α-hydroxylase enzymes, suggesting the potential for
1,25(OH)2D to have a local paracrine role [9], with it modulating immunity, cell growth and inflammation, cell
proliferation, cell differentiation and cell death [8]. To relate this to cognitive function, VDRs and
1-α-hydroxylase have been located in neurons and glial cells throughout the
central nervous system [10], particularly being expressed in regions for cognition
such as the hippocampus, hypothalamus, cortex and subcortex [11].
Vitamin D enters the cerebrospinal fluid
(CSF) by crossing the blood-CSF barrier in the plexus choroideus, and it enters
the brain by passive diffusion across the blood brain barrier and by carriers
in cerebral capillaries [11]. It has been stipulated to act as a
neurosteroid hormone [12], offering neuroprotection via a number
of mechanisms. In parallel to other tissues, 1,25(OH)2D has a role
in maintenance of calcium homeostasis [13], which prevents an increase in intracellular
free calcium and the consequential impairment of brain function and memory
formation, cell death [10], and stimulation of glutamate release [14]. It also prevents hyperparathyroidism, which has further
neuropsychiatric and cognitive effects [13]. In
addition, 1,25(OH)2D is thought to regulate of synthesis of
neurotransmitters such as dopamine, serotonin and γ-aminobutyric acid [11], increase acetylcholine availability by
altering choline acytransferase activity [13], and enhance innate antioxidant
pathways including upregulation of glutathione production [5], reducing reactive oxygen and nitrogen
species and free radical damage to DNA and membrane lipids [13]. It has been shown to promote
neurogenesis by controlling levels of neurotrophic agents including nerve
growth factor, neurotrophin-3, which protects nerve transmission, and glial
cell line derived neurotrophic factor, which affects the survival and
differentiation of dopaminergic cells [5], as well as inhibiting synthesis of nitric oxide to
reduce its associated neuronal cell alterations [10].
Further to this, 1,25(OH)2D
has been implicated to confer disease specific effects related to dementia. Low
vitamin D is thought to be a cardiovascular risk factor [15] so may offer protection against vascular lesions [12] and vascular
dementia. Whereas, in AD, stimulation of the innate immune system may
increase clearance of amyloid-β 42 peptide by macrophage phagocytosis [12], reducing its accumulation and the formation
of amyloid plaques. These plauqes cause inflammation, death of affected neurons and increased
phosphorylation of Tau proteins, which subsequently disassemble from
microtubules and aggregate to neurofibrillary tangles [13], causing neuronal degeneration, glutamatergic
excitotoxicity, excessive calcium entry into post-synaptic neurons, neuronal
necrosis and apoptosis [10], and cerebral atrophy. The brain to blood amyloid efflux
may also be enhanced to further this effect [11].
The vast number of proposed
neuroprotective effects of 1,25(OH)2D suggests that vitamin D
deficiency may be associated with risk of cognitive impairment and dementia.
However, there are inconsistencies within the literature. A recent study by Olsson
et al. investigated the association between
baseline vitamin D status, measured by plasma 25(OH)D and intake from diet and
supplements, with risk of dementia or cognitive impairment over an 18-year
period [2]. This review will discuss their conclusions within the
wider research to evaluate whether there is sufficient scientific evidence to
consider vitamin D deficiency as a risk factor for cognitive decline and
neurodegenerative diseases, and, consequently, whether there is rationale to
encourage increasing intake from dietary sources, sun exposure and the use of
supplements for this purpose.
Method
Study
population
Data from the third investigation in the
Uppsala Longitudinal Study of Adult Men was used, with age of subjects at this
examination being 69-74 years. Baseline anthropometric and lifestyle
measurements were taken.
Vitamin
D exposure
25(OH)D concentration was determined
from fasting plasma samples, with season of collection recorded. A 7-day food
record was completed with daily energy and nutrient intakes calculated with subjects
with extreme reported intakes being excluded. Vitamin D supplement use was
recorded and its contribution combined with dietary intake.
A genetic risk score (GRS) was calculated,
reflecting the number of alleles associated with higher 25(OH)D concentrations
in two single nucleotide polymorphisms.
Dementia
and cognitive impairment assessment
A mini mental state examination (MMSE)
was completed at baseline and at follow-up with low scoring subjects being
referred to a memory clinic for dementia diagnosis. Medical records were
reviewed to identify incident cases of AD, vascular dementia and all-cause
dementia.
Cognitive impairment was defined as a
decline of ≥3 points or a score at follow up of <25 points in the MMSE.
Statistical
analysis
Plasma 25(OH)D was analysed as
continuous variables and in predefined categories, and dietary vitamin D as
continuous variables and in tertiles. The GRS was assessed as a continuous
variable.
The associations between exposures and
AD, vascular dementia and all-cause dementia were analysed using Cox
proportional hazards regression, calculating time at risk from baseline
examination until dementia diagnosis, death or end of follow-up. Potential non-linear
trends between exposures and outcomes were investigated using restricted cubic
splines.
Results
Subject
characteristics
Total follow-up time was 18 years. 116
cases of AD, 64 cases of vascular dementia and 250 cases of all-cause dementia
were identified, and 16.4% of those completing the MMSE at follow-up were
classified as cognitively impaired. Only 408 of the 863 men to complete the
MMSE at baseline did so at follow-up.
Plasma 25(OH)D concentrations varied
between season of sampling.
Results
of statistical analysis
No association between plasma 25(OH)D
and incident AD, vascular dementia, all-cause dementia or cognitive impairment
was identified for either the continuous or categorised data, and no non-linear
associations were observed. Total vitamin D intake was not associated with any
of the outcomes.
The GRS was positively associated with
plasma 25(OH)D concentration but not with incident dementia or cognitive
impairment.
Discussion
The study by Olsson et al. concluded that there was no association between plasma
25(OH)D and incident AD, vascular dementia or all-cause dementia [2]. There is much diversity within the wider literature, although
a general skew against the findings of Olsson et al.. Littlejohns et al. observed
a monotonic association between plasma 25(OH)D concentration and both risk of
AD and all-cause dementia over 5.6 years of follow-up, as well as a link with
cerebrovascular pathology, risk of stroke, and neurodegeneration due to vascular
mechanisms [16]. Moreover, Annweiler et al. observed an association between
vitamin D at baseline and the onset of non-AD dementia within 7 years, although
no association with AD was found [17]. Similarly, MMSE score
has been shown to correlate with 25(OH)D levels, where individuals classified
as suffering from mild dementia had lower concentrations [18]. In contrast, although a smaller
hippocampal volume was found in those with vitamin D deficiency in the
Framingham Heart Study, no association with incident dementia or clinical AD
was observed [19], and Littlejohns et al. reported no difference in the
risk of development of cerebrovascular and neurodegenerative pathologies
associated with dementia such as worsening white matter grade, or risk of
ventricular or incident infarcts with serum 25(OH)D concentration [20]. A meta-analysis of five
observational studies reporting risk of dementia to be higher in those with
serious vitamin D deficiency compared to those with sufficiency, state that the
strength of the evidence was low due to the lack of consideration of all
important confounders [21]. Differences in adjustment in models
may therefore provide some explanation as to the variation in the
literature. In addition, there is a high
potential for reverse causality as, in more advanced cases, dementia can affect
the ability to undertake ADLs such as cooking and eating, and may result in
reduced sun exposure due to functional limitations [22]. This could imply that any positive
associations between 25(OH)D concentrations and dementia may in fact be a
reflection of poor health resulting from the causative diseases rather than
vitamin D deficiency forming part of their aetiology.
In addition, Olsson et al. did not observe an association between dementia and total
vitamin D intake from dietary and supplementary sources combined [2]. Annweiler et al. reported
dietary intake and sun exposure at midday to be inversely associated with AD,
with a 4 times decreased incidence for those with highest vitamin D consumption [7], whereas Rossom et al. reported no difference in incident dementia between placebo
and treatment groups, where a combined calcium carbonate and vitamin D3
supplement was provided containing 400IU of vitamin D3 [23]. It could be that dietary but not
supplemented vitamin D may confer neuroprotective effects, potentially due to
the effects of additional nutrients in vitamin D rich foods such as omega 3
fatty acids in oily fish, but also that cognitively impaired individuals may
provide unreliable data in food frequency questionnaires, causing inaccurate
results. Moreover, the supplemented dose of vitamin D may not have been
sufficient to affect cognitive functioning, and its combination with calcium
may have attenuated any effect that may result from supplementation of vitamin
D alone. The small number of studies considering intake of vitamin D as opposed
to plasma 25(OH)D concentration limits the ability to make any evaluation of
its potential direct role. Nonetheless, it is thought that 25(OH)D is a good
indicator of vitamin D stores due to its 2-3 week half-life, and reflects both
diet and sun exposure [8], meaning that taking supplements or
increasing dietary intake to optimise serum 25(OH)D may be of benefit, yet
supraphysiological doses may not.
The study by Olsson et al. also considered cognitive impairment as a secondary outcome,
although no association was observed with vitamin D intake or serum 25(OH)D
concentration [2]. The reasonably consistent conclusions within the
literature regarding this association, contrasting that made by Olsson et al., indicates that the study method
may have biased the data. Cognitive decline was measured as a change in MMSE,
but only in those healthy enough to attend examinations at baseline and
follow-up, potentially resulting in the ‘healthy cohort effect’, where subjects
included in this analysis were not representative of the true population.
The wider literature generally reports
lower levels of 25(OH)D to be associated with greater cognitive decline within
an elderly population in cross-sectional analyses [12][22][24][25][26][27][28], with a systematic review by van der
Schaft et al. found 18 of 25
cross-sectional studies to report worse performance in one or more cognitive
tests by those with low vitamin D [29]. However, the nature of these
studies means results may be explained by reverse causation, although the
effect may be less significant for those with mild cognitive impairment than
individuals with dementia [12]. In addition, many of these studies were conducted at a
memory assessment clinic or within those with subjective memory complaints,
suggesting the likelihood of diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment to be
higher than within the general elderly population. Despite this, the same
systematic review reported 4 of 6 prospective studies concluding that there was
a greater risk of cognitive decline at follow up for those with low 25(OH)D at
baseline [29]. The follow-up time for these was only
between 4 and 7 years, yet the study by Olsson et al. spanned 18 years. In this instance, it may be that taking
only one measurement of 25(OH)D at baseline resulted in misclassification of
individuals as serum concentration could vary considerably throughout an 18
year period.
To specify its effects on neuronal
function further, many studies have considered the association between serum
25(OH)D and domain-specific cognitive performance. Most frequently, low 25(OH)D
is related to poorer executive function [30][31][32], as well as processing speed [15][33] and visual perceptual skills [19].
However, the only associations observed with memory tend to be for working
memory [30][34][35],
and the broad findings are that there is no association with memory performance [32][33][36]. This suggests that any potential link
with dementias such as AD would be driven by non-amnestic cognitive decline [36].
In
contrast to the previously discussed correlations observed between serum
25(OH)D and cognitive performance, where lowest concentrations indicate worse
outcomes, two studies found have reported subjects in the middle categories of
serum 25(OH)D to have lower odds of cognitive impairment than both the highest
and lowest [37][38]. Granic et al. observed that those in the highest category of serum 25(OH)D
concentration were more likely to be taking prescribed vitamin D and have
osteoporosis [38], which may imply that those subjects
are potentially less healthy and at greater risk of cognitive decline than those
with moderate serum concentrations. Studies observing no
association between vitamin D status and cognition may have been similarly
affected, with both healthy and unhealthy individuals being categorised in the highest
group. With inadequate adjustment for covariates there would be the potential
for this to attenuate any positive association that may exist.
Finally,
Olsson et al. observed no association
between two genetic variants involved in vitamin D synthesis and incidence of
dementia or cognitive impairment [2]. The two alleles considered were in
the 7-dehydrocholesterol reductase gene and the vitamin D 25-hydroxylase gene.
Conversely, Pettersen et al. found
there to be no significant difference in vitamin D intake from food or
supplements, nor difference in sun exposure within subjects, yet serum 25(OH)D
concentrations varied. It was therefore suggested that other mediators of
vitamin D level such as VDR genotype may have a role in cognition [30]. Gezen-Ak et al. reported an association between the ApaI gene polymorphism
in the VDR ligand binding site and late onset AD [39], suggesting interindividual differences
in the response to 1,25(OH)2D may influence its effects. Moreover,
Kueider et al. observed an
association between cognitive decline and single nucleotide polymorphisms in
the GC gene, which encodes for the 25(OH)D carrier protein, [32], suggesting that individuals with
greater susceptibility to low bioavailability of vitamin D metabolites to
target cells may affect the association between serum 25(OH)D and cognitive
outcome. It may therefore be that a lack of adjustment for such polymorphisms
results in inconclusive or incorrect conclusions being obtained in both
cross-sectional and prospective studies.
Impacts
Discussion of the null findings in the
study by Olsson et al. [2] has found that there are great inconsistencies within the
literature regarding the role of vitamin D in dementia risk, and opposing but
potentially inaccurate observations with regards to association with cognitive
impairment. This, as well as the significant heterogeneity between studies as a
result of variation in diagnostic criteria, methods of assessing cognition, vitamin
D thresholds and choice of subjects [40], makes it challenging to reach a well
evidenced conclusion. The potential for reverse causality cannot be disregarded
as a significant factor in cross-sectional studies, which comprise the majority
of those published. Nonetheless, combining the observations in human studies
with findings from animal or in vitro
investigations into the neuroprotective mechanisms of vitamin D, it could be
suggested that there may be some benefit from ensuring all individuals at least
reach sufficiency [13].
It has been estimated that 1 in 2 older
adults suffer from vitamin D deficiency [11]. The cause is multifactorial, with lack
of sun exposure, low dietary intake, accelerated losses by medication induced
catabolism, impaired hepatic and renal metabolism, reduced bioavailability by
malabsorption and sequestration in fat being major mechanisms [41]. However, most significantly for the
elderly, the 25% reduction in epidermal 7-dehydrocholesterol compared to
younger adults greatly decreases the capacity for endogenous synthesis, as does
reduced sun exposure from functional decline [42]. Owing to there being few dietary
sources of vitamin D, mainly being obtained from oily fish such as salmon, mackerel
and tuna, and from vitamin D fortified dairy products, if a strong association
were to exist between serum 25(OH)D and cognitive function, regular
supplementation would be the most appropriate public health approach.
The rarity of vitamin D toxicity due to
the physiological regulation of its formation and metabolism [8], the proven role of vitamin D in skeletal health and
prevalence of deficiency within the demographic most at risk of cognitive
decline and dementia, means it could be said that all older adults should
consider taking a daily vitamin D supplement, as per current UK guidelines.
This would aim to prevent the adverse effects of deficiency such as
osteomalacia, osteoporosis and hyperparathyroidism [42], yet may also potentially minimise rate of cognitive
decline, onset of dementia and the resulting loss of independence.
[1] Linus Pauling Institute. (2014) Vitamin D. URL: http://lpi.oregonstate.edu/mic/vitamins/vitamin-D.
[2] Olsson, E., Byberg, L., Karlström, B., Cederholm, T., Melhus, H., Sjögren, P.
& Kilander, L. (2017). Vitamin D is not associated with incident dementia
or cognitive impairment: an 18-y follow-up study in community-living old men.
[3] Alzheimer's Research UK. (2017). 10
things you need to know about dementia. URL: http://www.alzheimersresearchuk.org/about-dementia/facts-stats/10-things-you-need-to-know-about-dementia/.
[4] Alzheimer's Society. (2016a). What is
dementia? URL: https://www.alzheimers.org.uk/info/20007/types_of_dementia/1/what_is_dementia.
[5] Buell, J. S. & Dawson-Hughes, B. (2008). Vitamin D and neurocognitive
dysfunction: Preventing "D"ecline? Molecular Aspects of Medicine, 29
(6), 415-422.
[6] Alzheimer's Society. (2016a). Dementia UK.
URL: https://www.alzheimers.org.uk/info/20025/policy_and_influencing/251/dementia_uk.
[7] Annweiler, C., Rolland, Y., Schott, A. M., Blain, H., Vellas, B., Herrmann, F.
R. & Beauchet, O. (2012). Higher Vitamin D Dietary Intake Is Associated
With Lower Risk of Alzheimer's Disease: A 7-Year Follow-up. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological
Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67
(11), 1205-1211.
[8] Haines, S. T. & Park, S. K. (2012). Vitamin D Supplementation: What's
Known, What to Do, and What's Needed. Pharmacotherapy,
32 (4), 354-382.
[9] Barnard, K. & Colon-Emeric, C. (2010). Extraskeletal Effects of Vitamin D
in Older Adults: Cardiovascular Disease, Mortality, Mood, and Cognition. American Journal of Geriatric
Pharmacotherapy, 8 (1), 4-33.
[10] Downey, C. L., Young, A., Burton, E. F., Graham, S. M., Macfarlane, R. J.,
Tsapakis, E. M. & Tsiridis, E. (2017). Dementia and osteoporosis in a
geriatric population: Is there a common link? World Journal of Orthopedics, 8
(5), 412-423.
[11] Annweiler, C., Allali, G., Allain, P., Bridenbaugh, S., Schott, A. M., Kressig,
R. W. & Beauchet, O. (2009). Vitamin D and cognitive performance in adults:
a systematic review. European Journal of
Neurology, 16 (10), 1083-1089.
[12] Annweiler, C., Schott, A. M., Allali, G., Bridenbaugh, S. A., Kressig, R. W.,
Allain, P., Herrmann, F. R. & Beauchet, O. (2010). Association of vitamin D
deficiency with cognitive impairment in older women Cross-sectional study. Neurology, 74 (1), 27-32.
[13] Annweiler, C. & Beauchet, O. (2011). Vitamin D-Mentia: Randomized Clinical
Trials Should Be the Next Step. Neuroepidemiology,
37 (3-4), 249-258.
[14] Annweiler, C., Rolland, Y., Schott, A. M., Blain, H., Vellas, B., Herrmann, F.
R. & Beauchet, O. (2012). Higher Vitamin D Dietary Intake Is Associated
With Lower Risk of Alzheimer's Disease: A 7-Year Follow-up. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological
Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67
(11), 1205-1211.
[15] Annweiler, C., Maby, E., Meyerber, M. & Beauchet, O. (2014).
Hypovitaminosis D and Executive Dysfunction in Older Adults with Memory
Complaint: A Memory Clinic-Based Study. Dementia
and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 37
(5-6), 286-293.
[16] Littlejohns, T. J., Henley, W. E., Lang, I. A., Annweiler, C., Beauchet, O.,
Chaves, P. H. M., Fried, L., Kestenbaum, B. R., Kuller, L. H., Langa, K. M.,
Lopez, O. L., Kos, K., Soni, M. & Llewellyn, D. J. (2014). Vitamin D and
the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Neurology,
83 (10), 920-928.
[17] Annweiler, C., Rolland, Y., Schott, A. M., Blain, H., Vellas, B. &
Beauchet, O. (2011). Serum Vitamin D Deficiency as a Predictor of Incident
Non-Alzheimer Dementias: A 7-Year Longitudinal Study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 32 (4), 273-278
[18] Peterson, A., Mattek, N., Clemons, A., Bowman, G. L., Buracchio, T., Kaye, J.
& Quinn, J. (2012). Serum vitamin d concentrations are associated with
falling and cognitive function in older adults. Journal of Nutrition Health & Aging, 16 (10), 898-901.
[19] Karakis, I., Pase, M. P., Beiser, A., Booth, S. L., Jacques, P. F., Rogers, G.,
DeCarli, C., Vasan, R. S., Wang, T. J., Himali, J. J., Annweiler, C. &
Seshadri, S. (2016). Association of Serum Vitamin D with the Risk of Incident
Dementia and Subclinical Indices of Brain Aging: The Framingham Heart Study. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 51 (2), 451-461.
[20] Littlejohns, T. J., Kos, K., Henley, W. E., Lang, I. A., Annweiler, C.,
Beauchet, O., Chaves, P. H. M., Kestenbaum, B. R., Kuller, L. H., Langa, K. M.,
Lopez, O. L. & Llewellyn, D. J. (2016). Vitamin D and Risk of Neuroimaging
Abnormalities. Plos One, 11 (5).
[21] Sommer, I., Griebler, U., Kien, C., Auer, S., Klerings, I., Hammer, R., Holzer,
P. & Gartlehner, G. (2017). Vitamin D deficiency as a risk factor for dementia:
a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC
Geriatr, 17.
[22] Perna, L., Mons, U., Kliegel, M. & Brenner, H. (2014). Serum
25-Hydroxyvitamin D and Cognitive Decline: A Longitudinal Study among
Non-Demented Older Adults. Dementia and
Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 38
(3-4), 254-263.
[23] Rossom, R. C., Espeland, M. A., Manson, J. E., Dysken, M. W., Johnson, K. C.,
Lane, D. S., LeBlanc, E. S., Lederle, F. A., Masaki, K. H. & Margolis, K.
L. (2012). Calcium and Vitamin D Supplementation and Cognitive Impairment in
the Women's Health Initiative. Journal of
the American Geriatrics Society, 60
(12), 2197-2205.
[24] Matchar, D. B., Chei, C. L., Yin, Z. X., Koh, V., Chakraborty, B., Shi, X. M.
& Zeng, Y. (2016). Vitamin D Levels and the Risk of Cognitive Decline in
Chinese Elderly People: the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological
Sciences and Medical Sciences, 71
(10), 1363-1368.
[25] Chei, C. L., Raman, P., Yin, Z. X., Shi, X. M., Zeng, Y. & Matchar, D. B.
(2014). Vitamin D Levels and Cognition in Elderly Adults in China. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
62 (11), 2125-2129.
[26] Moon, J. H., Lim, S., Han, J. W., Kim, K. M., Choi, S. H., Kim, K. W. &
Jang, H. C. (2015). Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D level and the risk of mild
cognitive impairment and dementia: the Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and
Aging (KLoSHA). Clinical Endocrinology,
83 (1), 36-42.
[27] Przybelski, R. J. & Binkley, N. C. (2007). Is vitamin D important for
preserving cognition? A positive correlation of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D
concentration with cognitive function. Archives
of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 460
(2), 202-205.
[28] Vedak,
T. K., Ganwir, V., Shah, A. B., Pinto, C., Lele, V. R., Subramanyam, A., Shah,
H. & Deo, S. S. (2015). Vitamin D as a marker of cognitive decline in
elderly Indian population. Annals of
Indian Academy of Neurology, 18
(3), 314-319.
[29] van
der Schaft, J., Koek, H. L., Dijkstra, E., Verhaar, H. J. J., van der Schouw,
Y. T. & Emmelot-Vonk, M. H. (2013). The association between vitamin D and
cognition: A systematic review. Ageing
Research Reviews, 12 (4),
1013-1023.
[30] Pettersen, J. A., Fontes, S. & Duke, C. L. (2014). The Effects of Vitamin D
Insufficiency and Seasonal Decrease on Cognition. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 41 (4), 459-465
[31] Pettersen, J. A. (2016). Vitamin D and executive functioning: Are higher levels
better? Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 38
(4), 467-477.
[32] Kueider, A. M., Tanaka, T., An, Y., Kitner-Triolo, M. H., Palchamy, E.,
Ferrucci, L. & Thambisetty, M. (2016). State- and trait-dependent
associations of vitamin-D with brain function during aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 39, 38-45.
[33] Buell, J. S., Scott, T. M., Dawson-Hughes, B., Dallal, G. E., Rosenberg, I. H.,
Folstein, M. F. & Tucker, K. L. (2009). Vitamin D Is Associated With
Cognitive Function in Elders Receiving Home Health Services. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological
Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64
(8), 888-895.
[34] Maddock, J., Geoffroy, M. C., Power, C. & Hypponen, E. (2014).
25-Hydroxyvitamin D and cognitive performance in mid-life. British Journal of Nutrition, 111
(5), 904-914.
[35] Assmann, K. E., Touvier, M., Andreeva, V. A., Deschasaux, M., Constans, T.,
Hercberg, S., Galan, P. & Kesse-Guyot, E. (2015). Midlife plasma vitamin D
concentrations and performance in different cognitive domains assessed 13 years
later. British Journal of Nutrition, 113 (10), 1628-1637.
[36] Kuzma, E., Soni, M., Littlejohns, T. J., Ranson, J. M., van Schoor, N. M.,
Deeg, D. J. H., Comijs, H., Chaves, P. H. M., Kestenbaum, B. R., Kuller, L. H.,
Lopez, O. L., Becker, J. T., Langa, K. M., Henley, W. E., Lang, I. A.,
Ukoumunne, O. C. & Llewellyn, D. J. (2016). Vitamin D and Memory Decline:
Two Population-Based Prospective Studies. Journal
of Alzheimers Disease, 50 (4),
1099-1108.
[37] Llewellyn, D. J., Langa, K. M. & Lang, I. A. (2009). Serum
25-Hydroxyvitamin D Concentration and Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
Neurology, 22 (3), 188-195
[38] Granic, A., Hill, T. R., Kirkwood, T. B. L., Davies, K., Collerton, J.,
Martin-Ruiz, C., von Zglinicki, T., Saxby, B. K., Wesnes, K. A., Collerton, D.,
Mathers, J. C. & Jagger, C. (2015). Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and cognitive
decline in the very old: the Newcastle 85+Study. European Journal of Neurology, 22
(1), 106-+.
[39] Gezen-Ak, D., Dursun, E., Ertan, T., Hanagasi, H., Guervit, H., Emre, M., Eker,
E., Oztuerk, M., Engin, F. & Yilmazer, S. (2007). Association between
vitamin D receptor gene polymorphism and Alzheimer's disease. Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine,
212 (3), 275-282.
[40] Etgen, T., Sander, D., Bickel, H., Sander, K. & Forstl, H. (2012). Vitamin D Deficiency, COgnitive Impairment and Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 33 (5), 297-305.
[41] Podd, D. (2015). Hypovitaminosis D: A common deficiency with pervasive
consequences. Jaapa-Journal of the
American Academy of Physician Assistants, 28 (2), 20-26.
[42] Annweiler, C. & Beauchet, O. (2014). Vitamin D in older adults: the need to
specify standard values with respect to cognition. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6.
Comments
Post a Comment